Cognitive Studies: Bulletin of the Japanese Cognitive Science Society
Online ISSN : 1881-5995
Print ISSN : 1341-7924
ISSN-L : 1341-7924
Volume 15, Issue 1
Displaying 1-20 of 20 articles from this issue
Foreword
Feature: The Models of Spatial Perception and Spatial Cognition
  • Toshio Watanabe, Haruyuki Fujii
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 3-5
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    Download PDF (378K)
  • Mark Wagner
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 6-21
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    Spatial perception and cognitive mapping are two areas of research that invite comparison since they both involve fundamental geometric concepts, and researchers have applied psychophysical methods to examine spatial judgments in both domains. The first part of article discusses research that assesses the degree to which spatial judgments conform to various geometric axioms. In general, both spatial perception and cognitive maps violate many of the most fundamental geometric axioms, including metric axioms, compactness, the Desarguesian postulate, and free mobility. Of the two, cognitive maps show the most striking violations of even the most basic geometric concepts, including symmetry, transitivity, and Riemannian manifoldness.
    The second and larger part of this article performs a meta-analysis on psychophysical judgments for size and distance, comparing judgments made in perception research to those found with cognitive maps. The most striking results are that cognitive maps produce much lower power function exponents and much lower coefficients of determination for size and distance estimation than perception does. The meta-analysis presented here also shows that a number of the same factors influence both types of distance estimates including judgment method, whether judgments occur inside or outside, and stimulus range. Cognitive mapping exponents also rise with acquisition time and decline with increasing environment size. Several multivariate analyses are performed to yield a set of general psychophysical equations to predict size and distance estimates.
    Download PDF (656K)
  • J. A. Aznar-Casanova, E. H. Matsushima, L. P. Zapata, N. P. Ribeiro-Fi ...
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 22-37
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    Distance from a stimulus, stimulus size and orientation with respect to the observer are relevant metric properties of visual space. However, whether and how these metrics are related to each other is not well known. Some studies have shown that estimates of egocentric distances are usually more accurate than judgments of exocentric distances, suggesting that there is dissociation between ‘localization’ judgments and ‘size’ judgments. Other investigations have revealed that the orientation of the stimulus influences the accuracy of size estimations.
    To better understand the relationships between these metric properties of visual space, we conducted an experiment to compare size perception as a function of orientation from viewer egocentric and exocentric frames of reference (FoR). Observers were instructed to draw two circles on a screen by clicking with a mouse when the screen was positioned on either the frontoparallel plane or the ground plane.
    From an egocentric FoR, egocentric distance and direction were processed asymmetrically, as knowledge of the particular combination of both size and orientation was always needed to compute visual direction. However, in terms of the accuracy of size estimates (S’/S) from an exocentric FoR, size and orientation were independent for all conditions. Therefore, the orientation did not need to be computed in order to compute the size. Regarding exocentric direction or orientation (α’/α), size and orientation were only dependent when the two points were presented successively. In conclusion, these findings suggest that orientation and distance always interact and that distance, visual direction or orientation require an egocentric cue.
    Download PDF (2131K)
  • Yoshiki Wakabayashi
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 38-50
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    The aim of this paper is to discuss the role of maps in the cognition of geographic space. The research topics were divided into (1) maps as a model of spatial cognition, and (2) maps as a source of spatial knowledge. As for the first topic, maps have become a conceptual model of the spatial cognition and enabled us to elaborate the model of cognitive processing of geospatial information, since Tolman (1948) devised the term of “cognitive map”. As a result, methods for measuring distortions in cognitive maps has been developed and applied by geographers. On the other hand, the role of maps as a source of spatial knowledge will become important as geographic information technologies spread among people. Specifically, advanced techniques for communicating geographic information through 3D view, animation, sound, and VR enabled by GIS lead to reconsideration of the dichotomy between direct and indirect sources of information. Hence, recent trend of digitization of geospatial information can change the role of maps in the cognition of geographic space.
    Download PDF (417K)
  • Atsuki Higashiyama
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 51-61
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    We investigated what information of maps is available while walking in a complex actual city. We drew nine maps by manipulating landmarks, geographic and egocentric orientations, and distances. Each of ninety participants attempted to walk a complex route that was indicated in a map handed by the experimenter. The number of participants who found their way or the latency that was needed for them to find it was compared among the map conditions. Landmark was very effective: As the number of landmarks increased from 0 to 143, the proportion of the participants who found the way increased from 30 to 100%, with a decrease of latency. Egocentric orientation was also effective: Half the participants with the left-right reversal map failed to find the way, although there were ample landmarks indicated in the map. Either geographic orientation or distance was less effective: Most participants succeeded with the fragmental map that was constructed by cutting a standard map into small pieces and sequentially arranging them along the route. We compared these outcomes with the results of studies done in virtual reality space.
    Download PDF (2233K)
  • Toshio Watanabe
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 62-74
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
  • Haruyuki Fujii
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 75-99
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    The model to implement the process of the decoding of a spatial description to its spatial image -- topology of the adjacent relations - has been created. The model enables the implementation of the system that updates and maintains the spatial model(s) constructed from the discourse. The model is trying to solve the four problems concerning how to resolve the reference of an anaphoric expression, how to handle transition of the utterer's point of view, how to detect and eliminate inconsistent information, and how to understand implicit and/or incomplete descriptions. The solution relies on the four assumptions derived from the literature survey. (1) Interaction between the system and the user helps the system to understand the meaning and reference of an ambiguous or vague description. (2) The transition of a point of view is calculated by tracing the point and perspective of the effective point of observation. (3) Consistency of the spatial relations is maintained by the combination of a spatial reasoning algorithm and the dual graph representation formalism and its rewriting rules. (4) Incomplete and/or implicit description is complemented based on knowledge about spatial relations and architecture.
    Download PDF (1110K)
  • Kazumi Nagata, Tomoya Miwa, Atsushi Osa, Makoto Ichikawa, Yoshiki Mizu ...
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 100-109
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    Psychological researchers have pointed out that as the distance between a viewer and an object increases, the perceived size of the object becomes larger than the size predicted using the linear perspective. This phenomenon is called “size constancy”. The purposes of this study are to determine a magnification-rate function for rendering a graphic image to fit it with size and distance impressions obtained in real space observation at a given distance, and to assess the validity of the magnification-rate function for computer graphic images. In the first experiment, we investigated the perceived size of an object placed at different distances, ranging from 1 to 10 m (short distance conditions), and from 4 to 120 m (long distance conditions). We found that the Z-ratio function (Thouless, 1931) can predict the perceived size for an object under the short distance conditions; however, it failed to predict the perceived size under the long distance conditions. Therefore, we applied a logistic function to predict the perceived size of an object under both the short and long distance conditions. In the second experiment, we asked observers to rate the fit of the size and distance of a computer graphic image with the perceived size and distance of objects in a real space. The size and distance of objects in the graphic images were determined using the magnification-rate function in terms of the logistic function, or the conventional linear perspective. We found that the observers gave higher scores for the images generated using the magnification-rate function than those generated using the conventional linear perspective. These results indicate that the magnification-rate function obtained in this study is more useful than the linear perspective in rendering computer graphic images to fit the size and distance of objects in the images with the real space observation.
    Download PDF (1575K)
  • Tadahiro Kitahashi, Tomomi Takezawa
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 110-119
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    In this paper, we discuss perceptive properties of people based on experiments by means of sequentially presenting pairs of pictures to an observer on a computer terminal.Observers are inquired whether they believe the subjects in the picture to have stayed in the same position or have changed positions, and if changed, whether they moved forward or backward and by how much. The precedent picture (called the standard picture) of each of paired pictures to be presented is taken of a person standing at fixed points of 9m, 13.5m and 27m in front of a camera. The antecedent one (referential one) is selected from the pictures that are taken of the same person who has slightly moved positions (0.5m, 1m, 1.5m in case of 9m standard position) in these three types of forward positions and three types of backward positions. We have some interesting results from the experiments concerning perception of forward/backward movement of a person. The results indicate that humans perceive a person to be approaching an observer more sensitively and accurately than that of a person departing from the observer. A result shows that the amount of perspective movement is closely related to the ratio of the size of person's images in the paired pictures. Additionally, observers estimate less of a change in movement at the farther points than from nearer points, and the background scene scarcely gives any effects on the estimation of the movement.
    Download PDF (932K)
  • Kayoko Ohtsu
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 120-133
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    Path integration is a navigation process in which self-velocity and self-acceleration over time are integrated to update one's positions. The sensory inputs used in path integration are classified into internal and external information. The internal information consists of physical senses from the vestibular system and kinesthetic sense (proprioception and efference copy). The external information is primarily a visual one, obtained from the optic flow. In Experiment 1, participants were asked to point out the start points after traveling 3 paths (containing 3, 5 or 7 turns & legs) in body-based (Internal) or vision-based (External) path integration tasks. Performance in Internal was better than that in External, particularly in the path with 5 turns & legs. In Experiment 2, participants learned the relative locations of 3 points along the path in both Internal and External and were tested on the basis of the pointing accuracy between points. Performance in Internal was more accurate than that in External only when tracing back on the path was needed to estimate directions. The results suggested that the internal information is vital to update the homing direction at turns where we are prone to lose it and helps us to acquire survey knowledge by providing homing directions.
    Download PDF (1848K)
  • Makoto Inagami, Ryuzo Ohno, Rieko Tsujiuchi
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 134-143
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    This study investigates the extent to which humans are aware of the space surrounding their bodies during daily locomotion. To capture this awareness in its natural state, our experiment adopted an indirect method that uses feelings of oppression and release caused by the surrounding environment. Participants continuously rated their feelings by turning a dial while walking along an outdoor route. The physical environment along this route was described in terms of the visible areas of buildings, trees, ground, and sky. Each area was measured from four different widths of view angles ranging from a limited view in front to a full 360° view. Analysis of the relationship between the ratings and measurements reveals that the feelings have the highest correlation with the 360° measurements. This result supports our hypothesis that humans are aware not only of the limited visual field but also of the entire surrounding space, including the space behind their bodies. Based on this finding, we discuss the traditional concept of spatial perception from an ecological perspective.
    Download PDF (1279K)
  • Tatsumi Kobayashi, Asuka Terai, Takenobu Tokunaga
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 144-160
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    Prior studies have indicated that on cognitive load, topological spatial terms are more easily selected than projective spatial terms as a selection mechanism, i.e., “near” tends to be preferred over “left” or “right”. To clarify the effect of geometric factors on spatial term selection, we performed a psychological experiment using 3D generated objects with sentences describing each object in spatial terms in Japanese, e.g., hidari (left), chikai (near), and tooi (far). Results showed that the characteristics for determining the acceptability rating of each spatial term varied by term and also by geometric factors such as the position or size of the object and the location of the distractor. We also performed two other experiments to investigate the effects of changing both point of view and the objects' size, with the goal of gaining a better understanding of some projective spatial terms in Japanese, mae (front), ushiro (back). These experiments confirmed the importance of both viewpoint and the objects' size as factors within the cognitive model. Finally, we discuss a new cognitive model for spatial term selection based on these findings.
    Download PDF (1050K)
Research Papers
  • Hiromichi Yokochi, Ichiro Nagasaka, Takaji Matsushima
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 161-173
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    In the studies of deductive reasoning, the experiments have always been conducted with tasks with just one logically correct answer. In this paper, three experiments examined how the reasoning process with underspecified tasks can be explained. Underspecified tasks are tasks with an arbitrary correct answer or with no correct answer at all. Experiment 1a and 1b showed that participants tend to choose the option that is more difficult to draw a conclusion than the other. Furthermore, the factors leading to such selection bias included the differences in the number of logical symbols contained in the reasoning process between options and the reductio ad absurdum process. Protocol analysis in Experiment 2 revealed that reductio ad absurdum has more impact on selection bias.
    Download PDF (1015K)
  • Kuninori Nakamura
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 174-187
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    Verbal probability statements such as “very certain” or “impossible” are often used to communicate information regarding uncertainty. This study aims to investigate how people estimate the “informativeness” of verbal probability statements. With regard to numerical probabilities (e.g., “30%”), Keren and Teigen (2001) proposed the “search for definitive predictions” principle; according to this principle, relatively high or low probabilities are preferred to medium ones because high or low probabilities denote the occurrence or nonoccurrence of a single outcome more strongly than they do medium ones. However, whether people adhere to this principle when they estimate the informativeness of the verbal probability statement remains an unresolved issue. Through two empirical studies, this study establishes a quantitative relation between verbal probability statements and their “informativeness,” which is similar to the principle of Keren and Teigen (2001). In addition, this study shows that the directionality of verbal probability (Teigen & Brun, 1999) also affects the informativeness judgment. Finally, this study formalizes the judgment regarding the informativeness of probability statements in terms of the information theory and argues that the “search for definitive predictions” principle can be interpreted as rational information estimation under the rarity assumption (Oaksford & Chater, 1994).
    Download PDF (715K)
  • Ryota Nomura, Shun'ichi Maruno
    2007 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 188-201
    Published: 2007
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    The purposes of the present study were to construct a humor elicitation model based on Comprehension-Elaboration Theory (Wyer & Collins, 1992), and to investigate the effects of Narrative Strategies on humor elicitation processes. 159 (79 male and 80 female) undergraduate and graduate students (18--25 yrs old) rated the model relevant variables after watching a videotaped RAKUGO performance. With structural equation modeling (SEM), it was confirmed that the model had generalizability and high intra-model consistency. The further results demonstrated that the performance with Narrative Strategies, compared to the one without the strategies, increased both comprehension and elaboration, which led to increase of humor.
    Download PDF (1205K)
  • Hideyuki Takahashi, Satoru Ishikawa, Takashi Omori
    2008 Volume 15 Issue 1 Pages 202-215
    Published: 2008
    Released on J-STAGE: October 30, 2009
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    It is thought to be an important brain function for us to modulate our cognitive state depending on recognition of interactive agents. From our behavioral experiment, we have shown that when subjects believed their interactive agent as a human being in a competitive game, specific behavioral tendency could be observed in comparison with a case that they believed their opponent as a computer and this tendency of subjects with autism spectrum disorder (ASD; they are thought to have some problems in social interaction) was different from that of subjects without ASD. We consider that this tendency is a reflection of cognitive modulation depending on recognition of interactive agents. To explain computational theory of this tendency, we propose a computational model that consists of change detection and state space switching evoked by the change of environmental nature. From this model, we reproduce the result of behavioral experiment by a computer simulation and try to discuss our computational model is useful to understand about the brain function for frequent human social interaction.
    Download PDF (1198K)
Literature Guide
Book Review
Short Book Review
feedback
Top