2020 Volume 5 Article ID: 20200025
Background: Sensory ataxia is a disorder of movement coordination caused by sensory deficits, especially in kinesthetic perception. Visual stimulus-induced kinesthetic illusion (KINVIS) is a method used to provide vivid kinesthetic perception without peripheral sensory input by using a video showing pre-recorded limb movements while the actual limb remains stationary. We examined the effects of KINVIS intervention in a patient with sensory ataxia. Case: The patient was a 59-year-old man with a severe proprioceptive deficit caused by left thalamic hemorrhage. During KINVIS intervention, a computer screen displayed a pre-recorded mirror image video of the patient’s unaffected hand performing flexion–extension movements as if it were attached to the patient’s affected forearm. Kinematics during the flexion–extension movements of the paretic hand were recorded before and after 20-min interventions. Transcranial magnetic stimulation was applied to the affected and non-affected hemispheres. The amplitude of the motor-evoked potential (MEP) at rest was recorded for the muscles of both hands. After the intervention, the total trajectory length and the rectangular area bounding the trajectory of the index fingertip decreased. The MEP amplitude of the paretic hand increased, whereas the MEP amplitude of the non-paretic hand was unchanged. Discussion: The changes in kinematics after the intervention suggested that KINVIS therapy may be a useful new intervention for sensory ataxia, a condition for which few effective treatments are currently available. Studies in larger numbers of patients are needed to clarify the mechanisms underlying this therapeutic effect.
Sensory ataxia is a disorder of movement coordination caused by sensory deficits, especially deficits in kinesthetic perception. Previous studies have shown that sensory ataxia occurs in patients with peripheral neuropathy1,2) and following thalamic stroke.3) Several studies have shown that rehabilitation can improve some sensory functions, such as tactile sensation and positional sense,4,5) but a systematic review by Doyle et al.6) concluded that there is insufficient evidence to support its effectiveness in improving sensory impairment. Furthermore, there have been very few reports regarding rehabilitation for sensory ataxia.2,7) Because sensory ataxia is caused by impairments in kinesthetic perception, providing appropriate kinesthetic feedback may help to improve sensory ataxia. However, if sensory pathways are severely impaired, peripheral sensory input may not be sufficient to adjust the movement trajectory. Notably, kinesthetic perception is also generated by top-down modulation within brain networks involved in motor planning and sensory–motor integration that do not rely on peripheral sensory afferent projections.8,9,10) Kinesthetic illusion induced by visual stimulation (KINVIS) is one potential method for providing kinesthetic perception without peripheral sensory inputs.9,10,11,12,13) To investigate this method, we developed a box-type virtual reality system to induce KINVIS, termed the KiNvis™ system. During KINVIS, a video of finger movement is shown on a computer screen located directly above an individual’s actual hand. This intervention induces body ownership and vivid kinesthetic perception of the “artificial body” on the screen.13) Furthermore, it selectively increases corticomotor excitability corresponding to control of the involved muscles12,13) and activates brain regions including the dorsal and ventral premotor cortices and left superior and inferior parietal lobules.10) These studies demonstrated that KINVIS generates neural activity similar to that observed during motor execution, even in the absence of the peripheral kinesthetic inputs. Consequently, KINVIS may be considered a stimulation paradigm for the embodied cognitive brain system.
A preliminary study revealed increased upper limb muscle activity and joint movement in stroke patients immediately following KINVIS.14) However, no study has observed the effects of KINVIS in stroke patients with sensory ataxia. The current study investigated whether visually induced kinesthetic perception in the absence of peripheral kinesthetic inputs affects sensory ataxia symptoms. To explore the feasibility of this technique, the kinematic, neurophysiological, and electromyographic changes after KINVIS were examined in a patient with sensory ataxia resulting from thalamic hemorrhage. Because kinesthetic perception plays a crucial role in motor execution and learning,1,15,16) we hypothesized that KINVIS may effectively reduce upper limb sensory ataxia.
A 59-year-old, right-handed, male stroke patient participated in this study. The patient had displayed sensory ataxia for 5 months in his paretic right hand as a result of severe proprioceptive deficits following a thalamic hemorrhage. His only neurological disease was stroke. The location of the brain lesion and the patient’s clinical characteristics are shown in Fig. 1 and Table 1, respectively. The patient had severe deep sensory loss and mild superficial sensory loss. The sensory subscore on the Stroke Impairment Assessment Set (SIAS) for the upper limb was 0 for position sense and 2 for touch sense. During the two-point discrimination test on the pulp of the index finger, the patient failed to distinguish the points even at a separation distance of 20 mm.
Computed tomography image of the patient 14 days after stroke onset showing left thalamic hemorrhage.
Characteristic | Value |
Age (years) | 59 |
Sex | Male |
Time since stroke (months) | 5 |
Diagnosis | Cerebral hemorrhage |
Lesion location | Left thalamus |
Motor function | |
Brunnstrom stage (I–VI) | |
UE | IV |
Finger | IV |
LE | III |
SIAS finger-function test (0–5) | 3 |
FMA UE (0–66) | 29 |
ARAT (0–57) | 20 |
MAS finger flexor (0–4) | 1 |
Sensory function | |
SIAS UE touch (0–3) | 2 |
SIAS UE position (0–3) | 0 |
Two-point discrimination | >20 mm |
UE, upper extremity; LE, lower extremity; SIAS, Stroke Impairment Assessment Set; FMA, Fugl-Meyer Assessment; ARAT, Action Research Arm Test; MAS, Modified Ashworth Scale.
The patient provided prior written informed consent to participate in the study in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The present study was approved by the local Ethics Committee of the Ibaraki Prefectural University of Health Sciences (approval number: e202). Written informed consent was also obtained from the patient for publication of this case report.
The patient was seated in a comfortable chair with his paretic right forearm on a table. Before the intervention, we filmed the participant executing hand flexion–extension movements (3-s flexion and 3-s extension) with the non-paretic left hand. During the intervention (KiNvis Therapy System™; Inter Reha, Tokyo, Japan), the patient remained fully relaxed and was instructed to keep his both hands still. A computer display showing the mirror image of the patient’s left hand was positioned over the right forearm. The position and size of the image were appropriately adjusted so that the paretic hand mimicked the feeling that an artificial hand belonged to the patient’s own body. KINVIS was performed for 20 min by repeatedly showing the 6-s video of the hand flexion–extension movement. The kinematics and muscle activity during hand flexion–extension movements and corticomotor excitability using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) were evaluated before and after the 20-min intervention.
A Magstim 2002 stimulator (Magstim, Whitland, UK) connected to a figure-of-eight coil was used to elicit motor-evoked potentials (MEP) from the paretic and non-paretic first dorsal interosseous muscle (FDI), the paretic flexor digitorum superficialis muscle (FDS), and the paretic extensor digitorum muscle (ED). The handle of the coil was positioned pointing backward and laterally at an angle of 45° from the midline to induce an anteromedial-directed current in the brain to trans-synaptically activate pyramidal neurons.17,18) Hot spots in each hemisphere that elicited the most consistent MEP amplitude from the contralateral FDI muscles were determined by moving the coil in 1-cm increments around the hand motor area. After determination of the hot spots, test stimuli were administered at least five times. The test stimulus intensities were set at 50% and 60% of the maximum stimulator output (MSO). The interval between the previous and subsequent stimuli was set to at least 8 s. To determine whether corticomotor excitability was changed by KINVIS, MEP amplitudes of the hand muscles in the resting condition were measured before and after the intervention. The location and intensity of the stimulation were the same before and after the intervention. Trials in which the root mean square of the electromyograph (EMG) activity exceeded 0.01 mV within 100 ms prior to TMS were rejected.19)
ElectromyographyThe skin at the electrode site was swabbed with alcohol and prepared using abrasive skin-prepping gel. Surface Ag–AgCl electrodes were placed over the paretic FDS, paretic ED, and bilateral first dorsal FDI. The positions of the electrodes were maintained throughout the experiment, including before and after the intervention. EMG signals were amplified (Neuropack MEB2300; Nihon Kohden, Saitama, Japan) at a gain of 1000 or 2000 and band-pass filtered at 5−5000 Hz. All signals were stored on a laboratory computer for offline analysis. The sampling frequency was set at 10 kHz during the TMS experiment and 2 kHz during the hand motor task.
Motor Task with Kinematic and EMG AnalysisThe patient put his paretic hand in a neutral position and performed the 6-s hand flexion–extension movement task (3-s flexion and 3-s extension) with the paretic hand four times. To focus on the kinematic analysis of hand movements, the experimenter fixed the patient’s distal forearm during the motor task. Reflective markers were placed on the patient’s index fingertip, distal interphalangeal joint, proximal interphalangeal (PIP) joint, metacarpophalangeal (MP) joint, and radial styloid process. The positions of the reflective markers were maintained throughout the experiment, including before and after the intervention. Hand flexion–extension movements were captured from above using a digital video camera (EX-FH100, 30 frames/s; Casio, Tokyo, Japan). The recorded images were digitized to obtain coordinates for the five reflective markers using a motion analysis system (Frame DIAS V; DKH, Tokyo, Japan). Two-dimensional (2D) coordinates for each marker were processed using a fourth-order, zero-lag, low-pass, Butterworth filter with a cut-off frequency of 6 Hz. The changes in flexion angles of the PIP and MP joints were calculated from the 2D coordinate data based on the reflective markers. For each hand flexion–extension movement, the flexion–extension angle ranges for the PIP and MP joints were calculated and averaged. Additionally, we used the indices of the total trajectory length and its rectangular area, which are often used to assess tremor of the trajectory of the hand movement and sway of the center of pressure in ataxic patients,20,21,22,23) to determine whether the extent of tremor of the trajectory of the fingertip during hand flexion and extension movement was different before and after the intervention.
Joint kinematic analysis during paretic hand movement in the motor task was performed simultaneously with EMG recordings. The EMG activity of the paretic FDI, FDS, and ED muscles were band-pass filtered (zero-lag Butterworth filter, 20−500 Hz), and EMG recordings were then full-wave rectified and smoothed using a fourth-order, zero-lag, low-pass filter (cut-off frequency: 6 Hz). The maximum value of the average muscle activity for 1 s during flexion–extension movement was calculated for each hand. The maximum value of four movements were averaged for each muscle. To further investigate the mechanism of kinematic changes resulting from the intervention, we calculated the co-contraction index24) between FDS and ED, which are the agonist and antagonist muscles of hand flexion–extension movements. This index is an indicator related to the coordination of a pair of agonist–antagonist muscles and is calculated as follows:
Co-contraction Index=EMGS/EMGL × (EMGS + EMGL)
where EMGS is the level of activity in the less active muscle, and EMGL is the level of activity in the more active muscle. The mean value of the co-contraction index at ± 500 ms of each maximum flexion and extension angle was calculated. Then, the mean values of the co-contraction index for the four flexion–extension movements were calculated.
Questionnaire Regarding Body Ownership and Illusory SensationFollowing the KINVIS intervention, the patient was asked to rate the sense of body ownership and illusory sensation during KINVIS using a 7-point Likert scale (−3, strongly disagree; 0, neither agree nor disagree; +3, strongly agree).
The patient rated his sense of body ownership during KINVIS as +2 (agree) and the subjective illusory sensation during KINVIS as +2 (agree).
Paretic Hand KinematicsThe MP and PIP joint angles during hand flexion–extension movements are shown in Fig. 2 The average flexion–extension angle ranges for PIP and MP joint movement were slightly increased following KINVIS (Table 2). The trajectories of the index fingertip during hand flexion–extension movements completed before and after KINVIS are shown in Fig. 3 Prior to KINVIS, we observed prominent tremor in the fingertip trajectory during hand flexion–extension movements typical of the sensory ataxia with severe proprioceptive deficits present in this patient. The total trajectory length and the rectangular area bounding the trajectory of the index fingertip decreased after KINVIS (Table 2).
Flexion angles of the metacarpophalangeal (MP) joint and the proximal interphalangeal (PIP) joint are shown before (left) and after (right) kinesthetic illusion induced by visual stimulus intervention. Also shown are electromyography (EMG) recordings of the activities of the extensor digitorum muscle (ED), the flexor digitorum superficialis muscle (FDS), and the first dorsal interosseous muscle (FDI) during hand flexion–extension movements (black lines, raw EMG; white lines, rectified and smoothed EMG). The co-contraction index between FDS and ED is also given.
Pre | Post | ||
Range of flexion–extension angle (°) | MP joint | 34.9 | 40.2 |
PIP joint | 85.2 | 90.9 | |
Total trajectory length (cm) | 280.0 | 252.5 | |
Rectangular area (cm2) | 139.2 | 108.6 | |
Maximum average EMG activity (µV) | Paretic FDI | 40.0 | 61.7 |
Paretic FDS | 8.0 | 11.6 | |
Paretic ED | 25.6 | 32.6 | |
Co-contraction index | Flexion phase | 36.1 | 78.4 |
Extension phase | 61.7 | 36.1 |
The trajectory of the index fingertip during hand flexion–extension movements before and after KINVIS.
Figure 2 shows the activities of the FDI, FDS, and ED muscles and the co-contraction index between FDS and ED muscles in the paretic hand during hand flexion–extension movements. Activation of the paretic FDI, FDS, and ED muscles increased after KINVIS (Table 2). The co-contraction index between FDS and ED muscles tended to be higher during the extension phase before the intervention but decreased after the intervention. Conversely, the co-contraction index during the finger flexion phase increased after the intervention (Table 2).
Corticomotor ExcitabilityTo examine corticomotor excitability before and after KINVIS, the amplitude of TMS-induced MEP was measured at two stimulation intensities (50% and 60% MSO). After KINVIS, the MEP amplitude for the FDI, FDS, and ED muscles in the paretic hand increased at both stimulus intensities (Fig. 4). Conversely, KINVIS did not consistently affect the MEP amplitude of the non-paretic FDI muscle.
Amplitude of the motor-evoked potential (MEP) at two stimulus intensities [50% and 60% of maximum stimulation output (MSO)] recorded for the paretic and non-paretic FDI muscles and the paretic ED and FDS muscles before and after KINVIS.
The results showed that, despite the patient’s severe loss of kinesthetic perception, KINVIS intervention elicited virtual kinesthetic perception. In addition, tremor of the fingertip trajectory, the total trajectory length, and the rectangular area bounding the trajectory during hand flexion–extension movements decreased following KINVIS, despite slight increases in range of motion of the MP and PIP joints. In parallel with these kinematic changes, increased hand muscle activity and temporal changes in co-contraction between finger flexor and extensor muscles were observed during hand flexion–extension movements. These results suggested that KINVIS intervention may contribute to a reduction of ataxic symptoms associated with sensory loss, which may be related to changes in the level or coordination of muscle activity of the hand muscles.
Few effective treatments are available for sensory ataxia associated with loss of kinesthetic perception. Therapeutic interventions targeting sensory perception and balance were reported to improve dynamic balance in patients with chronic neuropathy or multiple sclerosis with sensory ataxia.7) Caronni et al.2) observed an effect of standard inpatient rehabilitation on sensory ataxia in patients with peripheral neuropathy, reporting significant improvements in walking ability and dynamic balance. Although no previous study has demonstrated improvements in sensory ataxia symptoms per se, the present results suggest that providing KINVIS instead of peripheral sensory input, which may be severely impaired, is a novel, potentially useful intervention for sensory ataxia. A previous study examining the effects of KINVIS on paretic upper limb motor function in stroke patients indicated an increase in muscle activity during hand movement after the intervention.14) The results obtained here were consistent with those of this previous study. However, the previous study did not report the presence or absence of sensory impairment in stroke patients. Therefore, this is the first study to show that KINVIS intervention may be effective in improving motor function in patients with sensory deficits.
We speculated that changes in muscle coordination between hand muscles may be related to the mechanism underlying these kinematic changes in hand flexion–extension movements. In the current study, we calculated the co-contraction index between FDS and ED, which are the agonist and antagonist muscles during hand flexion–extension movements, as a measure of hand muscle coordination. We found that the co-contraction index between FDS and ED was higher in the finger extension phase before the intervention, whereas it was higher in the flexion phase after the intervention. We were unable to determine whether the change in the temporal pattern of the co-contraction index after the intervention was the result of EMG activity approaching a normal pattern. However, because KINVIS intervention resulted in parallel changes in both the kinematics during finger flexion–extension movements and the temporal pattern of the co-contraction index of the agonist and antagonist muscles of these movements, the changes in temporal pattern of the co-contraction may have had some positive effect on the kinematic changes.
However, we cannot exclude the possibility that the kinematic changes in finger flexion–extension movements may have been caused not only by changes in muscle coordination but also by an increase in muscle activity itself. We found that the muscle activity in the paretic hand during hand flexion–extension movements increased after KINVIS, as reported previously.14) In parallel with these increases in muscle activity in the paretic hand muscles, we showed that MEP amplitudes in the paretic hand increased after the intervention, whereas there were no consistent changes in MEP amplitudes in the non-paretic hand. Therefore, KINVIS may have selectively increased corticomotor excitability in the damaged hemisphere, resulting in increased muscle activity during paretic hand movements. KINVIS-induced neurophysiological changes in normal individuals include increased frontoparietal network activity10) and increased corticomotor excitability as measured using TMS.12,13,14) However, it was unclear whether KINVIS increased corticomotor excitability in stroke patients. This is the first report showing that KINVIS could increase corticomotor excitability as well as muscle activity in a stroke patient. Further investigations are needed to determine whether the increases in corticomotor excitability and muscle activity are associated with improvements in hand kinematics in patients with sensory ataxia.
KINVIS has inherent advantages over other methods used to induce kinesthetic illusion, such as tendon vibration and mirror visual feedback (MVF). Tendon vibration induces a kinesthetic illusion by increasing Ia-afferent activity.25,26) This technique requires intact afferent transmission that activates the somatosensory cortex, which is not activated during KINVIS.10) KINVIS does not require intact input from the paretic hand, making it more appropriate for use in cases of sensory ataxia associated with severe proprioceptive deficits.
MVF also induces a kinesthetic illusion and increases corticomotor excitability in healthy patients.27,28) Further, it reportedly increased excitability of the affected primary motor cortex (M1) in stroke patients after a single 30-min session.29) However, movement of the unaffected hand is required for MVF. Movement of the unaffected hand may increase M1 excitability in the unaffected hemisphere. In theory, this may inhibit increased M1 excitability in the affected hemisphere due to interhemispheric inhibition.30) The KINVIS system in the present study used a pre-recorded mirror image video of the non-paretic hand, allowing the generation of a kinesthetic illusion in the paretic hand without simultaneous movement of the non-paretic hand. Further, it selectively increased M1 excitability in the affected hemisphere. Therefore, KINVIS may circumvent potential issues arising from interhemispheric inhibition. Further studies are required to verify differences in neurophysiological effects between different kinesthetic illusion techniques, including comparison of the strength of interhemispheric inhibition between MVF and KINVIS.
The present case study had several limitations. First, although the patient in this study presented with sensory ataxia caused by a severe proprioceptive deficit, we cannot completely exclude a potential effect of cerebellar ataxia resulting from dentate–rubro–thalamic tract injuries. Second, we did not assess the changes in sensory function associated with the intervention. Indeed, previous studies examining the effects of KINVIS in stroke patients also did not investigate the changes in sensory function associated with the intervention.14,31) Consequently, we cannot exclude the possibility that improvement in hand movement was affected by changes in sensory function. This is an important issue that is closely related to the mechanisms underlying the effects of KINVIS, and further studies regarding this issue are required.
In this feasibility study, KINVIS therapy resulted in kinematic improvement of hand movements in a patient with sensory ataxia following stroke. Our results indicated that KINVIS is a new potentially useful intervention for sensory ataxia caused by severe kinesthetic deficits that currently have few effective treatment options. Future studies with larger sample sizes using not only kinematic data but also indexes of muscle coordination, brain activity, and sensory and motor function are needed to determine whether KINVIS interventions are effective in ameliorating symptoms of sensory ataxia and to clarify the underlying mechanisms.
This research was supported by the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development (AMED; grant number 18he0402255h0005) and the IPU (Grant-in-Aid for Encouragement of Scientists).
FK is the founding scientist of Connect Inc., a commercial company set up in October 2018 for the development of rehabilitation devices. This company does not have any relationship with the device or setup used in the present study. FK received license fees from Inter Reha Co., Ltd. The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.