視聴覚教育研究
Online ISSN : 2433-0884
Print ISSN : 0386-7714
ISSN-L : 0386-7714
8.9 巻
選択された号の論文の14件中1~14を表示しています
  • 原稿種別: 表紙
    1978 年 8.9 巻 p. Cover1-
    発行日: 1978/03/31
    公開日: 2017/07/18
    ジャーナル オープンアクセス
  • 原稿種別: 目次
    1978 年 8.9 巻 p. Toc1-
    発行日: 1978/03/31
    公開日: 2017/07/18
    ジャーナル オープンアクセス
  • 原稿種別: 付録等
    1978 年 8.9 巻 p. App1-
    発行日: 1978/03/31
    公開日: 2017/07/18
    ジャーナル オープンアクセス
  • 牟田 博光, 坂元 昂, 新井 郁男
    原稿種別: 本文
    1978 年 8.9 巻 p. 1-16
    発行日: 1978/03/31
    公開日: 2017/07/18
    ジャーナル オープンアクセス
    In this study the effectiveness of the CATV system in instructing penmanship by an expert was compared with that of conventional type of lecture by an unskilled teacher. The total of 53 children were selected as subjects from school A and B, 17 and 36 respectively. 36 children of the school B were given the conventional instruction by an unskilled teacher at first. Then, after several days, some of the same subjects were exposed to the TV program for Japanese language of good quality. 17 children of school A were only exposed to the TV instruction. After the instruction every subject was expected to submit his (or her) work to the experimenter. The letters to be written were designated as chinese characters of "誠意". The style of writing was evaluated by three different evaluators according to the several criteria for good writing. The total score for each subject was calculated by using these scores. Three experimental groups, i.e. school B with only conventional instruction, school B with both conventional and TV instructions and school A with only TV instruction, were compared by these scores. Although the scores are slightly different by evaluators, the total score for school A was higher than the other two experimental groups. School B with two kinds of instructions follows and then school B with only conventional instruction. As to the scores by each criterion, the same tendency was obtained and the differences among these groups were significant in most cases. The total standard scores of each evaluator were calculated and analyzed by descriminant analysis. These scores discriminate the three experimental groups. Especially school A was discriminated fairly well with the other two groups. These results show the effectiveness of CATV instruction by a skilled teacher over the conventional instruction by an unskilled classroom teacher.
  • 佐賀 啓男
    原稿種別: 本文
    1978 年 8.9 巻 p. 17-35
    発行日: 1978/03/31
    公開日: 2017/07/18
    ジャーナル オープンアクセス
    As a fundamental study of audio-visual communication, this study was designed to determine the relationships of verbal and pictorial presentation of stimuli to their associative meanings. Wicker (1970) found no significant difference in the number of verbs between verbal and pictorial presentation through continuous association. Saga (1974)'s study of free discrete association showed that the ratio of verb responses was higher in the printed word than in the line drawing condition. This study has the aim to reconsider the discrepancy between these two studies. Subjects were 30 female college freshmen and 30 female sophomores. Through continuous association, verbs were collected from a list in which 10 sets of words and line drawings were presented. The second session of this procedure was conducted after a week with the converse conditions of words and pictures in order to examine the effect of the previous experience. Mean numbers of responses as a whole were not significantly different between word and picture conditions. The analysis of variance showed significant effects of session, class, and item, but did not of the presentation modes. The rank order correlation coefficient was significant between word and picture condition. Consequently, it can be said that the item (stimulus) itself was more effective than the presentation modes (words or pictures) in the number of continuous responses of verbs. The increase in the number of responses from the first to the second session was significantly greater in the first word and the second picture condition than in the reversed order condition. This seems to suggest that more free responses to words at the first session followed by the unique responses to picture produced more associations at the second session. The results of factor analyses using the number of responses as an index showed no factors such as "word factor" or "picture factor". However, in some items there were considerable variances of factor loadings between the two presentation modes, which suggest that the characteristics of the drawing patterns of pictures, or the ways of selecting criterial attributes might be a possible variable of associative meaning of pictures.
  • 長田 順子
    原稿種別: 本文
    1978 年 8.9 巻 p. 37-63
    発行日: 1978/03/31
    公開日: 2017/07/18
    ジャーナル オープンアクセス

    This study tries to examine the possible relationships among cognitive tempo, reflection-impulsivity, and reading ability of foreign language, Japanese, on a perceptual level. The previous study by Kagan (1965) on reflection-impulsivity and reading ability among first graders is to be retested. 27 reflective, 24 impulsive, 13 fast-accurate, 15 slow-inaccurate English speaking children of ages 10-12 were tested for accuracy in reading Japanese with the following three hypotheses: 1) Reflectives would make fewer mistakes than impulsives in reading Japanese sentences orally. 2) Reflectives would make fewer mistakes in finding correct meanings of Japanese words in Japanese-English dictionary than impulsives. 3) Reflectives would resort to a more effective strategy in consulting dictionary than impulsives. It was also intended to see some possible relationships between types of reading errors and reflection-impulsivity. Reflection-Impulsivity (R-I) measures were obtained by MFFT (Matching Familier Figures Test originally designed by J. Kagan and revised by Furu et. al.), and their correlation coefficients with reading tests scores were computed with Japanese vocabulary scores partialled out. Two kinds of reading tests, oral reading tests and dictionary tests, were conducted on each of 79 children. Each child was asked to read aloud four Japanese sentences and his/her reading was taperecorded to be analysed later. Before reading, each subject (S) was instructed by an administrater (A) in English to "try to guess" or "skip" words if he didn't knowhow to read. On reproducing the tapes, (A) checked all the errors or omissions made by S according to seven types of errors on the check sheet and timed the seconds for S to finish reading each of the four sentences. Reading errors were assigned to one of the following 7 categories. No.1. Mispronunciation - S sounds out the world but does not pronounce it correctly. No.2. Meaningful substitution - S does not attempt to sound out the word and substitutes a new word that retains the meaning and coherence of the sentence. No.3. Nonmeaningful substitution - S does not attempt to sound out the word and substitutes a word that has neither phonetic nor semantic similarity to the original and alters the meaning of the sentence. No.4. Self-correction - S initially misreads a word but corrects himself. No.5. Intentional omissions - S does not read a word due to inability to decode it. No.6. Impulsive omissions - S does not read a word by mistake. No.7. Intrusion - S adds a word or a phrase that is not in the text. Categories No.2 and No.3 were pooled to form Kagan's impulsive errors, since Kagan regarded them as impulsive errors and found positive relationships with impulsivity. Categories No.2 and No.5 were pooled as Goodman's reflective errors since Goodman emphasises that meaningful substitutions are not reading errors but marks of understanding and that the more proficient a reader is, the greater the proportion of semantically acceptable miscues. No.5 was included in reflective errors, because intentional omissions are considered as results of reflection according to the instructions. Categories No.3, No.4, No.6 and No.7 were pooled as Goodman's impulsive errors. No.1 was excluded because mispronunciation is due to the inability to read rather than an impulsive error. Thus Kagan's impulsive errors (meaningful and nonmeaningful substitution) are contrasted with Goodman's impulsive errors (non-meaningful substitution, self-correction, impulsive omissions and intrusion) and Goodman's reflective errors (meaningful substitution and intentional omissions). For the dictionary test, a child was handed a Japanese-English dictionary and told, "I want you to look up some Japanese words in this dictionary and let me know." Five Japanese words spelled in Kanji, hirakana, and roman letters,

    (View PDF for the rest of the abstract.)

  • 篠原 文陽児, 岡本 敏雄, 村本 紘, 中川 敏夫
    原稿種別: 本文
    1978 年 8.9 巻 p. 64-93
    発行日: 1978/03/31
    公開日: 2017/07/18
    ジャーナル オープンアクセス
  • 浜野 保樹
    原稿種別: 本文
    1978 年 8.9 巻 p. 94-101
    発行日: 1978/03/31
    公開日: 2017/07/18
    ジャーナル オープンアクセス
  • 原稿種別: 付録等
    1978 年 8.9 巻 p. App2-
    発行日: 1978/03/31
    公開日: 2017/07/18
    ジャーナル オープンアクセス
  • 原稿種別: 付録等
    1978 年 8.9 巻 p. App3-
    発行日: 1978/03/31
    公開日: 2017/07/18
    ジャーナル オープンアクセス
  • 原稿種別: 付録等
    1978 年 8.9 巻 p. App4-
    発行日: 1978/03/31
    公開日: 2017/07/18
    ジャーナル オープンアクセス
  • 原稿種別: 付録等
    1978 年 8.9 巻 p. App5-
    発行日: 1978/03/31
    公開日: 2017/07/18
    ジャーナル オープンアクセス
  • 原稿種別: 表紙
    1978 年 8.9 巻 p. Cover2-
    発行日: 1978/03/31
    公開日: 2017/07/18
    ジャーナル オープンアクセス
  • 原稿種別: 目次
    1978 年 8.9 巻 p. Toc2-
    発行日: 1978/03/31
    公開日: 2017/07/18
    ジャーナル オープンアクセス
feedback
Top