詳細検索結果
以下の条件での結果を表示する: 検索条件を変更
クエリ検索: "Doubt,"
16,534件中 1-20の結果を表示しています
  • 小野 晃任, *菅原 研
    ロボティクス・メカトロニクス講演会講演概要集
    2019年 2019 巻 1P2-H02
    発行日: 2019年
    公開日: 2019/12/25
    会議録・要旨集 認証あり

    Foraging behavior to collect objects distributed in the field is one important task suitable for multirobot system. For effective performance, proper cooperation should be introduced into the robots. In this study, we propose a novel method for cooperative collective behavior of swarm robots. Once the robot finds an object, it stops there and broadcasts its position to other robots. The robots which receive the signal try to move towards the signal source and to search for another object. In our proposal, we introduce a “

    doubt
    point”, which is incremented when the robot fails to find another object. If the point exceeds given limitation, the robot ignores the signal even if it detects the signal from other robots. In this paper, we confirm the effectiveness of this algorithm in several different types of object distribution by computer simulation.

  • *杉本 洋一, 荒川 雅生, 清水 裕子, 石丸 昌彦
    設計工学・システム部門講演会講演論文集
    2020年 2020.30 巻 2103
    発行日: 2020年
    公開日: 2021/06/29
    会議録・要旨集 認証あり

    This study aimed to verify whether there were linkages between“people who performed/non- performed as a

    doubt
    person in team exercises”and“individual Jung’s psychological types”.The subjects of this survey were 335 (265 men and 70 women,mean age±SD=18.7±0.8 years).Statistical tests (Fisher exact test,Residual analysis) were performed on them. The results suggested that there were certain linkage’s pattern between them under the condition of “not high enough situation of team psychological safety”.Firstly,there was a statistically significant linkage between “people who performed”and “extraversion” (p < .01), whereas there was a statistically significant linkage between “people who non-performed”and “introversion” (p < .01).Secondly,there was a statistically significant linkage between“people who performed”and “extraversion and sensing” (p < .05), whereas there was a statistically significant linkage between“people who non-performed”and “introversion and sensing” (p < .05).Thirdly,there was a statistically significant linkage between“people who performed”and “extraversion and thinking” (p < .05), whereas there was a statistically significant linkage between“people who non-performed”and “introversion and feeling” (p < .05).While the results should be treated as a tentative, they can be used as a clue of preventing phenomena concerning “failure to indicate” in teams that challenge to achieve a goal through collaboration.

  • 松長 有慶
    印度學佛教學研究
    1964年 12 巻 2 号 844-835
    発行日: 1964/03/31
    公開日: 2010/03/09
    ジャーナル フリー
  • *杉本 洋一
    設計工学・システム部門講演会講演論文集
    2023年 2023.33 巻 1305
    発行日: 2023年
    公開日: 2024/03/25
    会議録・要旨集 認証あり

    To leverage the diversity of team members and enhance decision-making quality, a “

    Doubt
    -person” (an individual who resists peer pressure and expresses doubts or opposing views) must be present, depending on the situation. This study surveyed students from the 2019 academic year enrolled in the “Teamworking Exercise” course at the Faculty of Creative Engineering, Kagawa University. The course introduced an educational program centered on “collaborative design via Type Utilization Education, drawing from Jung’s Type Theory.” This examination assessed whether students exhibited “
    Doubt
    -person” behavior during the exercises. Responses from 234 students in the analysis were statistically analyzed to investigate the association between exhibited “
    Doubt
    -person” behavior and the hypothesized psychological type. Findings indicated that differences in proactivity, attributed to “differences in judging function type,” had a more sustained impact than those from “differences in attitude type.” From an educational perspective, to increase “
    Doubt
    -person” without decreasing their numbers, emphasis should be placed on explanations appealing to those with “hypothetical type of thinking” and those that mitigate the averseness for individuals with “hypothetical type of feeling.” Awareness of this difference is necessary and effective concerning training and securing a “
    Doubt
    -person.”

  • ―ブランド・ナショナリズムを越えて―
    興津 妙子, 高山 敬太
    国際開発研究
    2024年 33 巻 2 号 125-135
    発行日: 2024/11/30
    公開日: 2025/01/22
    ジャーナル フリー

    This paper explores the ethical issues and potentialities of EDU-Port Japan, a public-private platform launched in 2016 by the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science, and Technology (MEXT) to export “Japanese-style of education” to emerging and developing countries.

    We evaluate the ethical dimensions of this initiative through the lens of Noriyuki Hashimoto's concept of ethics in international educational development (Hashimoto 2019). Drawing on post-colonial scholarship, Hashimoto (2019) contends that ethicality of the EDU-Port can only be realized when the full scope of “Japanese-style of education” —both its positive and negative aspects—is transparently disclosed. Furthermore, he argues that the act of transferring Japanese education abroad must reject the cultural imperialism that perpetuates notions of superiority and the false universalization of self, allowing instead a space for self-

    doubt
    (toinaoshi).

    Our analysis revealed that, in addition to the official objectives of contributing Japanese economic growth and enhancing soft-power, EDU-Port policymakers intended to use EDU-Port as a means of renewing self-understanding and practicing self-

    doubt
    . While these intentions align with what Hashimoto (2019) describes as practices of ethicality, our investigation suggests that the label of “Japanese-style education” is often used arbitrary and superficially within EDU-Port pilot projects, functioning as a “floating signifier.” This selective presentation serves to highlight the “goodness” of Japanese education while downplaying its negative aspects, thereby bolstering brand-nationalism. Many pilot project implementers experienced false universalization of self during education transfer, with only a few experiencing self-
    doubt
    and learning from the “Other,” during the first phase (2016-2020) .

    Finally, we argue that EDU-Port should be transformed to be truly horizontal and mutual learning initiative, embracing an alternative approach to educational development, that builds on Japan's traditional of field-based assistance while overcoming brand nationalism.

  • 竹市 嘉紀
    トライボロジスト
    2024年 69 巻 9 号 601
    発行日: 2024/09/15
    公開日: 2024/09/15
    ジャーナル 認証あり
  • 宗 兼利
    印度學佛教學研究
    1971年 19 巻 2 号 641-642
    発行日: 1971/03/31
    公開日: 2010/03/09
    ジャーナル フリー
  • 藤吉 慈海
    印度學佛教學研究
    1965年 13 巻 1 号 346-350
    発行日: 1965/01/31
    公開日: 2010/03/09
    ジャーナル フリー
  • FUJIO YOSHIMURA, KANJI HARUMIYA, RYUZO ITO, NAOZUMI SUZUKI
    Endocrinologia Japonica
    1964年 11 巻 2 号 101-111
    発行日: 1964/06/30
    公開日: 2011/01/25
    ジャーナル フリー
    Sixty male rats of Wistar-Imamichi strain castrated at the 45 days of age were decapitated at the 10, 30, 50, 55, 60, 180, 240, 300th and 421st postoperative day respectively. The anterior lobe of pituitary, adrenal and thyroid were investigated histologically. Commonly, in the control intact rats of various ages, the thyrotroph (T) was polygonal and the gonadotroph (G) spherical, together stainedwith PAS; with PAS-methylblue stain, T was PAS-purple, while G PAS-red; with aldehyde fuchsin stain, T positive but G negative. The significant proliferation of hypertrophied basophile cell was the main event following the prolonged castration. They belong exclusively to the castration cell. After the 150th postoperative day, most of them were transformed into so-called signet-ring cells containing the huge colloid mass; when they fell into an exhaustion, they gained the PAS-purple tincture, which may be concerned with the high density of the ground substance of the cell-body. In a moment T disappeared in the pituitary, the thyroid would be naturally expected to be atrophic. Nevertheless, the fact being contradictory to this expectation, thyroid provided no atrophic pattern, but rather the vigorous chronical response at the same level as by TSH administration. A expedited picture was no longer seen in adrenal cortex. From these observations, a question could naturally arise on the fixed classification of G and T. The monism is here tenable concerning the TSH and GTH secretion from a kind of basophile cell in the anterior lobe of pituitary.
  • Toyomi SANO, Yutaka IIDA, Shunichi YAMAGISHI
    Proceedings of the Japan Academy
    1966年 42 巻 10 号 1197-1201
    発行日: 1966年
    公開日: 2006/09/12
    ジャーナル フリー
    Employing microelectrodes and isolated rabbit right atrium, the sites of conduction block were examined by increasing the potassium content of the Tyrode solution.
    The conduction block occurred first in the whole left atrium and the right atrial portions closer to the atrioventricular groove. Next conduction block appeared in the extension of the upper branch of the crista terminalis near the atrial septum. In the third stage of block it was seen on the atrial septum side and in the adjacent caval region, including the septal side of the upper and lower branches. In the terminal stage of block the excitation wave from the sinus node could reach the crista terminalis, but it could not go much farther.
    The fact that the sinus node area was found to be the most resistant in these experiments suggests that the so-called sino-atrial block observed clinically in the electrocardiogram may actually be intra-atrial block.
  • 池田 幸恭
    青年心理学研究
    2010年 22 巻 57-67
    発行日: 2010/08/31
    公開日: 2017/05/22
    ジャーナル フリー
    This study aimed to clarify factors of resistance against gratitude to parents in adolescents. Participants were junior-high school, high-school, and university students (N=191) who answered about their frequency of resistance against gratitude to their parents and described the factors of such resistance. Around half of the participants felt resistance against gratitude to their parents. High-school students tended to feel more gratitude to mothers than did junior-high school and university students, and female students tended to feel more gratitude to their father than did male students. Secondly, eleven factors of resistance against gratitude to parents were identified: shyness of gratitude to parents, self-immaturity, excessive intervention by parents, low concern about oneself from parents, coerciveness into parents' sense of values, criticism of parents' personality, aversion to parents, blaming parents as roots of oneself,
    doubt
    that parents love oneself,
    doubt
    about equality of parents' love between siblings, and
    doubt
    that parents' loving is not egoism. Finally, three categories were obtained from these eleven factors: insufficient maturation, dissatisfaction with parents, and
    doubt
    of parents' loving oneself.
  • 岡本 仁宏
    年報政治学
    2011年 62 巻 1 号 1_11-1_48
    発行日: 2011年
    公開日: 2016/02/24
    ジャーナル フリー
      The word “国民”, ‘kokumin’, is used too much without a close examination and this word should be abandoned in many contexts, even not all.
      The use of “国民” is almost pervasive in Japanese political and legal discourses. “国” means the state or the country, and “民” means the people. Usually “国民” can be translated to “the people” or “the nation”.
      Historically “国民主権”, national sovereignty, was used for substitute of the popular sovereignty to make obscure which has sovereign, the Emperor or the people. In the Constitution of Japan, “We, the Japanese people,” is translated to “日本国民”, which can mean Japanese nationals or Japanese nation. It is not easy to change the constitution, however, the ambiguous “国民” should not be used as much as possible or at least should be interpreted as the people “人民” and as including all the people who live long enough under Japanese sovereignty.
      As EU conceptualized EU citizenship, many countries have been forced to redefine each concept of nationality and citizenship. Japan has a large number of non-citizens residents, which include Koreans who lost Japanese nationality in 1952. Recently voting rights of those people in local elections has become one of the big political issues. We argue that separation of citizenship and nationality is necessary for not only living with those people but also constructing multi-layer political units with each level of citizenship. “国民”, the national people, as the subject of the sovereign should be transformed to “人民”, the people, constituted with “市民”, citizens, of course, who do not mean exclusively the national citizens.
      国民, and maybe “nation” also, is the concept of the modern, the historical stage dominated by sovereign nation-states. We should scrutinize necessity of these words carefully for the emerging next stage.
  • 平原 晃宗
    印度學佛教學研究
    2003年 51 巻 2 号 675-678
    発行日: 2003/03/20
    公開日: 2010/03/09
    ジャーナル フリー
  • 大串 文隆
    日本内科学会雑誌
    1998年 87 巻 2 号 217-222
    発行日: 1998/02/10
    公開日: 2008/06/12
    ジャーナル フリー
    病原微生物の進入に対しマクロファージ,好中球やリンパ球を中心に生体防御機能が作動しこれらの排除が行われる.この過程では菌の種類,感染の時期などにより異なった種々のサイトカインが関与する.生体防御機構には炎症性サイトカインが主となる非特異的なものとTh1サイトカイン, Th2サイトカインが主体となる特異的防御機構がある.これらのサイトカインが複雑なネットワークを形成し病態形成に深く関与している.
  • 佐々木 大悟
    印度學佛教學研究
    2003年 52 巻 1 号 212-214
    発行日: 2003/12/20
    公開日: 2010/03/09
    ジャーナル フリー
  • 大松 龍昭
    印度學佛教學研究
    1995年 43 巻 2 号 545-547
    発行日: 1995/03/25
    公開日: 2010/03/09
    ジャーナル フリー
  • 瓜生津 隆雄
    印度學佛教學研究
    1953年 2 巻 1 号 189-190
    発行日: 1953/09/30
    公開日: 2010/03/09
    ジャーナル フリー
  • 留岡 清男
    心理学研究
    1928年 3 巻 3 号 307-322
    発行日: 1928年
    公開日: 2013/05/21
    ジャーナル フリー
    INTRODUCTION
    Looking over the history of “Denkpsychologie” I found it strange that the problem of the process of
    doubt
    has received but slight attention at the hands of research workers. Being interested in the morphological aspect of the consciousness of
    doubt
    , I made an attempt, in my present investigation, to describe and analyse the consciousness of
    doubt
    .
    The present investigation of the process of
    doubt
    was carried on in the Psychogical Laboratory of Hosei University during last winter, under the direction of Prof. Kido and Prof. Mitsui. The experiments were divided into two parts. In the first part I observed the following three simpler mohtal processes:
    1. The discrimination of two tactual impressions given by aesthesiometer on the velar side of the fore arm.
    2. The comparison of two distances perceived by a subject as he bends twice his forearm; the standard stimulus being 30cm and the comparison stimuli of various lengths.
    3. The judgment of a tone whether the middle tone is heard or not, three tones being given, higher, midde and lower. In this experiment a monocord was employed to give tones.
    To each of these experiments I applied both the method of minimal changes and that of right-and-wrong cases.
    In the second part of my experiments I made an observation of much more complex mental processes; that is, the arrange- ment and classification of various cards, all of which are different in color, form, mark or in their combinations.
    RESULTS
    Although the magnitudes of stimuli necessary for the occur- rences of the consciousness of
    doubt
    were various with different subjects, still I was able to find a regular relation between the magnitude of stimuli and the occurrence of the consciousness of
    doubt
    . In the discrimination of two tactual impressions upon the velar side of the forearm, all the subjects were observed to fall in
    doubt
    when they came into certain range, that is, between a point at which they began to feel indistinctly the given impres- sion as a single one and a point at which they felt it clearly as two separate ones. In the comparison of two distances, all the subjects were observed to fall in
    doubt
    when the comparison stimuli were equal to, or slightly longer or shorter than, the standard stimulus. In the judgment of a tone whether it was heard as the middle tone or not, all the subjects were observed to fall in
    doubt
    when the tone required to be judged was equal to, or slightly higher or lower than, the objectively middle pitched tone.
    It may be noticed here that these regular occurrences of the conscioness of
    doubt
    do not mean there is no possibility of other irregular occurrences of it under various conditions. Under what conditions do the subjects come necessarily to be doubtful ? What kinds of
    doubt
    can there be found ? In order to throw a light on these questions, let me look into all the records of the introspection of the subjects.
    The occurrences of the consciousness of
    doubt
    seem to depend on the following conditions:-
    1. The inadequateness of impressions,-
    a. Intensity and pain-sensation.
    In the discrimination of two tactual impressions Sub. T. introspected, “At first I thought I had perceived only a single impression. But as its intensity was very weak I can not tell it precisely.”
    In the judgment of a tone whether it was heard as the middle tone or not, Sub. Y. introspected, “I thought it was a middle tone as I had expected to be. But as its intensity was very strong, I can not tell it precisely.”
    b. Some accidental sensations.
    In the comparison of t we distances Sub. T. introspected, “At the moment the arm-movement came to an end I heard a noise at the top of the arm-holder. By the aid of the noise, I could immediately visualize the distance.
  • 赤羽 幸兵衛
    日本鍼灸治療学会誌
    1968年 17 巻 1 号 37-40
    発行日: 1968/01/15
    公開日: 2011/05/30
    ジャーナル フリー
  • 竹井 邦[ひこ]
    日本作物学会紀事
    1942年 14 巻 1 号 71-78
    発行日: 1942/09/30
    公開日: 2008/02/14
    ジャーナル フリー
feedback
Top