Research Journal of Budo
Online ISSN : 2185-8519
Print ISSN : 0287-9700
ISSN-L : 0287-9700
Volume 53, Issue 2
Displaying 1-9 of 9 articles from this issue
Original research paper
  • Takahiro NABEYAMA, Noriaki USUI, Motoki OKUMURA
    2021 Volume 53 Issue 2 Pages 55-71
    Published: March 31, 2021
    Released on J-STAGE: April 28, 2021
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS

    This study investigates how spatiotemporal conditions of interpersonal distance and timing in movement initiations influenced decision-making and actions for offence and defense in kendo. We also intend to present verifiable data on the problem of how two players separate from tsubazeriai in matches. Participants were top level players in Japanese university kendo clubs. In the experiment, participants were either given the role of “the first player”, who initiates movements, or “the second player”, who initiates movements after the first. The participants performed each trial as if in a real match from distances of 150, 175, 200, 225, 250, and 275 cm. We analyzed two trends in their decision-making and actions, one of which was the “ease of active striking,” meaning that they were able to initiate movements from a strike rather than defense in each trial. The other trend was the “ease of striking”, meaning participants could strike in each trial and were not only confined to defense. The results showed that it was easier for the first players than the second players with regards to “ease of active striking” and “ease of striking”. In both results, the differences between the first and the second players were extremely clear at a distance of 150 cm and were very clear at 175 to 250 cm distances and almost disappeared at 275 cm. In total, the first players also had a greater frequency of striking success (ippon) than the second players. These results indicated that movement initiation distance and timing changed reaction and movement times in both offence and defense, and also changed the degree to which the first and the second players’ decision-making and actions gave them an advantage. This is because the reaction and movement times required for offence and defense became shorter if the two players were closer to each other. In addition, the first players’ active movements caused the second players to react passively making it easier for the first players to initiate an attack.. Therefore, at close distances, offence became easier while defense became more difficult, and the first players gained a advantage while the second players were placed at a disadvantage. It can be concluded that the first players gain an advantage and the second players become disadvantaged in terms of offence when they are at close distances of 150 to 250 cm. These findings should be useful in combat sports such as kendo for the coaching of decision-making and actions, as well as for making the rules fairer on offense and defense.

    Download PDF (1104K)
  • Takamasa SAKABE, Hideaki TAKAI
    2020 Volume 53 Issue 2 Pages 73-83
    Published: 2020
    Released on J-STAGE: April 28, 2021
    Advance online publication: December 23, 2020
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS

    The purpose of this study was to understand the information processing ability of karatedo players from behavioral and physiological indices using a choice reaction test. Participants were seven men who belonged to a university karatedo club. The experiment was as follows. The participants carried out a choice reaction test for image stimulation by pressing a button with either hand. They were instructed to press a button with their right hand when an upper punch image was shown and with their left hand when a middle punch was shown. In addition, the image was partially occluded by a spatial occlusion paradigm. The three conditions for image occlusion were “no-occlusion condition”, “upper body occlusion condition”, and “lower body occlusion condition”. and were conducted in a random order. The reaction time from the presentation of the stimulus to the button press and the gazing area were measured as behavioral indices, and the latency and amplitude of P300 to the stimuli were measured as physiological indices. As a result, it was shown that the gazing area during task performance was mainly at the upper body. The reaction time also showed that the upper body occlusion condition was significantly delayed compared to the lower body occlusion condition, which implies that the information obtained from the upper limbs and the trunk is an important clue when judging the punching technique. Furthermore, since the P300 amplitude in the middle punch was significantly increased compared to the upper punch, it is speculated that the middle punch was judged with a higher degree of confidence. From the above, it is suggested that karatedo players perform reaction tasks with efficiently collected information inherent in the movements, especially for the middle punch, based on their past experience and knowledge.

    Download PDF (719K)
  • Shinichiro SATO, Toshihiro TAKEZAWA, Naoya MAEKAWA, Kiyoshi ITO, Masah ...
    2021 Volume 53 Issue 2 Pages 85-102
    Published: 2021
    Released on J-STAGE: April 28, 2021
    Advance online publication: January 06, 2021
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS

    The purpose of this study is to elucidate effective tactics in judo matches for the visually impaired. This study investigates and analyses the score ratio and score acquisition rate of visually impaired judo athletes in competition. In visually impaired judo the rules of non-disabled judo are modified into four phases. The first phase starts immediately after the referee announces “kumi-kata” to just before the referee announces “hajime”. The second phase begins right after the referee announces “hajime” to just before the first throwing technique. The third phase is from the start of a throwing technique to the end of a throwing technique. The fourth phase is from the end of a throwing technique to the end of a katame-waza.

    For this study data was compiled from 218 matches from domestic and international visually impaired judo championships. The tactical behavior of 1,375 nage-waza techniques were analyzed in sequence relating to kumi-kata combinations. The analysis was divided into the following categories: the controlled grip (CG); the presence or absence of the switch grip (SWG); the re-grip (RG); nage-waza; katame-waza, including the transition from a nage-waza; the penalties incurred during a match; and the match result. Statistical analysis of the data was performed using a chi-square test and a Fischer’s exact test with Microsoft Excel and R.

    The results obtained show the ratio of kenka-yotsu score was significantly higher (χ2=5.837, df=1; p<0.05) than ai-yotsu in the CG. The score ratio was significantly higher (χ2=8.468, df=1; p<0.05) when SWG tactics were applied. The score ratio of the osaekomi-waza was significantly higher in the nage-waza with a score, and the score ratio of the shime-waza was significantly higher than the nage-waza without a score (χ2 = 9.416, df = 4, p <0.05).

    The conclusions of this study indicate that in the overall match strategy where the first to second phases are performed with CG as kenka-yotsu, but without RG, it is tactically advantageous to use SWG only if your opponent avoids kenka-yotsu. In the fourth phase, however, it is clear that nage-waza while shifting to osaekomi-waza when a score is available, or shifting to osaekomi-waza or shime-waza when a score is not available, increases the rate of victory. It is hoped that the knowledge obtained from this study can be utilized not only to further develop the teaching methodology of both techniques and tactics for visually impaired judo athletes, but also to improve the overall competitiveness of judo.

    Download PDF (1918K)
53rd Congress of Japanese Academy of Budo
Headquarters project keynote lecture
Judo Specialist Subcommittee
Kendo Specialist Subcommittee
Kyudo Specialist Subcommittee
Naginata Specialist Subcommittee
Specialist Subcommittee of Budo for the Disabled
feedback
Top