I. Purpose and Method
In the present study the author examines the developmental sequences of number concept and clarifies the congnitive characteristics of number in retarded children. The experiments tried hereconsist of the following three fields.
1) Experiment I, to analyse the results from items concerning both mathematical abilities (i. e. naming number words, countings, the addition and subtraction operations, etc.) and the logical operations of number (i. e. conservation).
2) Experiment II, to examine the effects of the verbal instructions for countings, on the recognition of equivalences of perceptually different sets.
3) Experiment III, to practise an experimental education for equivalences of groups and examine the developmental aspects of acquiring conservation of number. We set the IS group of children (to be verbally instructed with therules of conservation-reversibility and the like-), and the US group(not to be instructed with them) in this training in order to study the children's self-instructive behaviours. Here children are generally taught equivalences of sets (consisting of the discrete and the continuous) by one-one correspondences and counting concrete objects. Tests are administered four times: pre-test, middle-test, post-test, and retention-test.
Subjects are 111 retarded children (morons) of special classes (IQ: 60, 79, MA: 3-9) and 60 normal children (in total). Main findings may be summarized as follows.
II. Results
1) There are three stages in the developmental sequences of the number concept of retarded children.
The first: (up to about MA 4). Equivalence is responded to on the basis of perceptual uniformity. Thus a child can recognize equivalences only when the elements and the arrangements of two groups are the same.
The second: (from MA 4 to MA 7). Visual perceptual cues of a group seem to become half eliminated. A child can generalize any elements as a class “1”, count objects and recognize equivalences only in the same arrangements.
The third: (after MA 7 or 8). The relationship between groups begins to be conceptualized, some addition and subtraction operations are understood, and then conservation can be acquired.
2) Retarded children may be shorter of selfinstructions for countings and too much influenced by their perceptions to compare the class exactly, while their levels of success can be raised to a degree by the instructions for cou-ntings that an experimenter gives.
3) The acquisition of conservation in the retarded depends upon their levels of learning experiences in mathematics.
4) Normal children, attaining the level of counting, can formulate for themselves, or solve the conservation problems using efficiently the instructions given verbally. However, ratarded children, who are at nearly the same levels of MAs and mathematics with the normal, can not do that. Retarded children, who can solve the addition and subtraction operations, acquire the concept of conservation while they receive that training. Then in this point, conservation is to be acquired faster to a degree by the experimental education. Still an essential requirement for children to acquire the concept (conservation) is to experience each step to it as definitely as possible.
View full abstract