The Japanese Journal of Educational Psychology
Online ISSN : 2186-3075
Print ISSN : 0021-5015
ISSN-L : 0021-5015
Volume 32, Issue 3
Displaying 1-12 of 12 articles from this issue
  • Shigeru Nakano
    1984Volume 32Issue 3 Pages 164-173
    Published: September 30, 1984
    Released on J-STAGE: February 19, 2013
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    Many studies on play in childhood have examined only one of the three main play dimensions: play partner, type of interaction, and type of play activities. However, it seems that child's play involves various combinations these dimensions.
    In this study, interrelationships between dimensions, and individual differences in these dimensions were examined. Each of 25 preschool children was observed by two observers every ten sec. for a period of 100 seconds. Each play activity was coded in each of the above three dimensions by using a checklist. Each child was observed 8 times during an 8 week-period.
    Results showed that the results based on a single dimension consisted of only one special combination of the dimensions. There were also “meaningful” exceptions. The combinatorial patterns of the dimensions were changeable depending on the situational demands.
    However, it was also found that there were some preferences for a special type of the combinatorial pattern based on individual differences. From this result, a developmental model based on the situational changes in the combinatorial patterns and the individual differences was proposed.
    Download PDF (1627K)
  • Naoko Yoshida
    1984Volume 32Issue 3 Pages 174-181
    Published: September 30, 1984
    Released on J-STAGE: February 19, 2013
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    The purpose of this study was to investigate the relations between labeling effects and eye movements on perceptual matching tasks. Two topics of special interest were studied: (1) the effect of training by labeling meaningful words, and (2) the process of perceptual transfer.
    In the experiment a 2×2 factorial design was used, which incorporated labeling training (yes or no) and matching tasks (pre-test or post test). Ss were 48 young children (5 years old) divided into two groups: an experimental and a control group. Stimuli consisted of 3 similar drawings for the labeling session, and 6 pairs of drawings made from the previous drawings, for the test sessions. Labels of the drawings were “inu” (dog) and “risu” (squirrel). One label was assigned to 2 drawings.
    Eye movements were measured by the ophthalmography which could take a photograph of a corneal reflected light. Movements were only measured while Ss were looking at a pair of drawings on the rear screen in both test sessions.
    Main results were as follows.
    1. In the experimental group Ss were influenced by the labeling training: 78.8% of them answered “same” to mismatched pairs. In the control group all of Ss answered correctly.
    2. Eye movements of Ss in the experimental group were also influenced by labeling training. When they answered incorrectly, fixations and its duration were decreased significantly. However, the number of shift movements between drawings did not show any change.
    The above results showed that meaningful labeling by children have effects on the perceptual matching tasks; it also showed a change in their eye movements.
    Through these findings we may conclude that a) the transfer of answers involves a change of eye movements and b) eye movements may be influenced by a symbolic factor.
    Download PDF (1301K)
  • Masato Sasaki, Akira Watanabe
    1984Volume 32Issue 3 Pages 182-190
    Published: September 30, 1984
    Released on J-STAGE: February 19, 2013
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    The purpose of the present study was to investigate the origin of spontaneous writinglike finger movements (“kusho”) in Japanese. To test the hypothesis that the “kusho” behavior was based on Kanji learning experience, people of three different culture areas (Japan, China, and other non-Kanji culture areas) were compared.
    In experiment 1, 21 Chinese subjects engaged in the Kanji grapheme integration task, had their appearance rate of “Kusho” behavior and the cognitive function examined. The results showed that all Chinese did Kusho behavior mixed with a certain cognitive function to facilitate the integration of Kanji graphemes (FIG. 1).
    In experiment 2, 83 Japanese college students, 21 Chinese college students, and 23 other non-Kanji culture students engaged in a three-English-word task (forward spell reading, backward spell reading, and word completion), the appearance rate of “Kusho” and its cognitive function were examined. The results of appearance rate showed that 60-80% Japanese students did “Kusho” behavior in each task, and non-kanji culture students did not show any “Kusho” behavior. On the other land, Chinese students showed a large difference in task condition (FIG. 4) The analysis of functional role of “Kusho” behavior in English word completion task (TABLE 1) showed that Japanese students revealed similar effect as in a Kanji grapheme integration task as for non-Kanji culture students, the “Kusho” proved to have rather disturbance effect (FIG. 3).
    From these findings, cultural backgrownd of “Kusho” behavior and its specificity for Japanese were discussed.
    Download PDF (1652K)
  • Etsuko Hatano
    1984Volume 32Issue 3 Pages 191-205
    Published: September 30, 1984
    Released on J-STAGE: February 19, 2013
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    The purpose of this study was to examine the developmental process of pragmatic negation from the sensorimotor interaction to the early linguitic communication.
    The data for this case study were the longitudinal diary records of my daughter A, from her birth to 24 months of age. The data for analysis were 396 nonverbal and verbal episodes of pragmatic negation.
    Resultant findings:
    (1) The development of media used for pragmatic negation
    The earliest emergence of media she used was crying with nondirective activities. The second was exclusive direct actions for an object or person, the third was the symbolic use of ritual headshaking gesture, and the last was the verbal negation. The abovementioned negative media were not only the change from gesture to language, but the later media were combined to the earlier media, then building more effective communications.
    (2) The acquisition of head gestures
    Nodding and shaking the head are a type of symbolic behaviour similar to affirmative/negative particles, and the establishment of these gestures was at 1: 1.
    (3) The process of the functional differentiation of pragmatic negation
    It could clearly be seen that function preceded from in the development of negation. In this study five-functional categories of negation were suggested: 1. exclusive rejection, 2. rejective expression of request for supportive action, 3. denial, 4 nonexistence, 5. prohibition.
    Rejection was the first function in which negations were used for, the first topics of negation were concretely present in the child's immediate world of activity and transcended the here and now only when negative comments came up. About a year later, other types of negation such as denial, non-existence, prohibition appeared. In the later negation contexts, the topic of negation had transcended on the spot.
    The negation acquired later was based on one's judgment about unfulfilled expectation or presupposition. The more mature use of unfulfilled expectation provided an important insight into the development of pragmatic negation. So, the developmental sequence of negation from emotional to declarative was confirmed.
    (4) Form-Function Mapping
    Some lexical items, iya (iyayo, iyada), attspu, metts (a baby-talk), nai, ii, chigau, were used to signal exclusive rejection. Lexical item nai (inai) signified nonexistance, nai expressed denial, and dame (metts) indicated prohibition. In early stage, nai was overregularized to almost all functions to negation.
    (5) The developmental strategies of negative lexicon
    Strategy 1. To intend negation, the specialized vocal must be used.
    Strategy 2. The negative operator is used independently and negates the whole proposition.
    Strategy 3. The negative operator has fixed location, and in Japanese, it is put at the last part of the utterances.
    Strategy 4. To combine the words, the negative operator is only added to other linguistic items.
    Strategy 5. To combine the words, other linguistic items are inflected, and then added to the negative operator.
    Strategy 6. To combine the words, other linguistic items (nouns and nominal adjectives) are followed by the negative form janai.
    Download PDF (2417K)
  • [in Japanese]
    1984Volume 32Issue 3 Pages 206-211
    Published: September 30, 1984
    Released on J-STAGE: February 19, 2013
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    Download PDF (1058K)
  • [in Japanese]
    1984Volume 32Issue 3 Pages 212-216
    Published: September 30, 1984
    Released on J-STAGE: February 19, 2013
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    Download PDF (886K)
  • [in Japanese]
    1984Volume 32Issue 3 Pages 217-222
    Published: September 30, 1984
    Released on J-STAGE: February 19, 2013
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    Download PDF (973K)
  • [in Japanese]
    1984Volume 32Issue 3 Pages 223-227
    Published: September 30, 1984
    Released on J-STAGE: February 19, 2013
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    Download PDF (826K)
  • [in Japanese]
    1984Volume 32Issue 3 Pages 228-232
    Published: September 30, 1984
    Released on J-STAGE: February 19, 2013
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    Download PDF (924K)
  • [in Japanese]
    1984Volume 32Issue 3 Pages 233-237
    Published: September 30, 1984
    Released on J-STAGE: February 19, 2013
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
    Download PDF (799K)
  • 1984Volume 32Issue 3 Pages 245-
    Published: 1984
    Released on J-STAGE: February 19, 2013
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
  • 1984Volume 32Issue 3 Pages 245a-
    Published: 1984
    Released on J-STAGE: February 19, 2013
    JOURNAL FREE ACCESS
feedback
Top