心理学研究
Online ISSN : 1884-1082
Print ISSN : 0021-5236
ISSN-L : 0021-5236
39 巻, 6 号
選択された号の論文の6件中1~6を表示しています
  • 馬場 雄二
    1969 年 39 巻 6 号 p. 289-298
    発行日: 1969/02/10
    公開日: 2010/07/16
    ジャーナル フリー
    The present study was designed to investigate the phenomenon of serial order from the considerations of the reading as well as the writing habits of Japanese, i.e., from the perceptual-motor aspect. In order to select subjects equalized on the point, a psycho-motor pretest was administered for once a week fifteen times to 139 woman college freshman students in an introductory psychology course. Eased on the results of pretest nine groups of 5 to 10 Ss each were selected.
    Three different types of stimulus patterns were employed; digits, filled and open circles and Hiragana letters (a kind of Japanese ordinary alphabet). Each pattern contained six elements in a horizontal row. Patterns were matched in pairs as far as possible, and were divided into A-series and B-series. They were presented in random order at an exposure duration of 100msec. Ss were requested to report what they saw, in verbal responses (V. R.) to A-series, and in writing responses (W. R.) to B-series.
    Experiment I was conducted to discover how stimuli were recongnized when they were presented at the left fixation point (L. P.), or at the right fixation point (each, N=5). No significant difference of recognition scores was found between L. P. and R. P., nor between V. R. and W. R.. However, recognition characteristics were significantly different between L. P. and R. P. (See Fig. 1). Experiment II dealt with the effects of left-right and right-left writing directions on recognition at the center fixation point (C. P.). Recognition scores of two groups (each, NN=5) were almost equal. In left-right directions, ordering of letter stimuli was found less flexible than that of digit stimuli. Probably, letters were several times more redundant than digits (See Table 2 and 3). Experiment III was designed to study the effects of horizontal and vertical writing directions on recognition at C. P. (each, N=5) (Japanese have these two reading and writing habits; left-right and up-down). Both recognition scores and characteristics of two groups were almost the same (See Fig. 2).
    Experiment IV was an attempt to throw a light on the relation between the pretest results and the accurately recognized stimuli (each, N=5). Recognition scores of the superior group in the pretest were significantly higher than those of the inferior.
    An attempt was made to find out how stimuli were recognized by V. R. and W. R. groups (each, N=10) in Experiment V, where A-series patterns were presented to R. P. and B-series to L. P.. Though scores of the W. R. group were almost equal for L. P. and R. P., scores of the V. R. group for R. P. in digits and letters were significantly less than for L. P. (See Fig. 3).
    With the results of Experiment I and II, two retinal effects (the end effect and the fixation-point effect) and two central processes (the sliding process and the gearing process) were postulated. The end effect means that the primitive figure-ground segregation occurs more rapidly for stimuli located at both ends, because three sides of them were completely open. The fixation-point effect implies that any stimulus subtended about 2° visual angle is more sensitively received because it falls on fovea centralis. The sliding process is assumed to act rapidly, roughly and driftingly, while the gearing process slowly, steadily and in a fixed direction.
    A paradoxical phenomenon observed in previous studies that the right visual field dominates when stimuli are presented on either side, while the left field dominates when stimuli are shown on both sides, was interpreted by the microgenetic point of view discussed above. Based on the results of Experiment III, IV, and V, it was pointed out that the sliding process works mainly in visual areas, or rather spatially, while the gearing process works mainly in motor areas, or rather temporally. It was suggested that an autonomic integration of “Habit” is a spatiotemporal
  • 石原 静子, 藤田 統
    1969 年 39 巻 6 号 p. 299-307
    発行日: 1969/02/10
    公開日: 2010/07/16
    ジャーナル フリー
    Previous studies have shown an induction phenomenon, i.e. a considerable decrement in the variability of behavior and a facilitation of its running response, in the semi-circular maze when reinforcement was presented after or between nonreinforced trials. The present series of experiments were designed to test whether or not a similar effect would be shown in a T-maze, and to determine what kind of stimulus was used as a cue for choice. For these purposes, percentages of spontaneous alternations and the running time were measured during the conditioning, extinction, and reconditioning periods.
    Experiment I: Thirty-three hungry rats were divided equally into three groups: Group 0, Group 11, Group 41. After exploration and pretraining, all Ss were given 11 successive free-choice trials per day for 4 days in the T-maze (the conditioning period). All choices were rewarded during this period. On the following day, Group 0, Group 11 and Group 41 received 0, 11 and 41 successive nonreinforced trials respectively (the extinction period), and then all Ss were given 11 reinforced trials (the reconditioning period).
    The percentage of alternations was significantly higher than chance level during the conditioning period. However, the greater the number of nonreinforced trials during the extinction period, the significantly greater the decrement of alternation in percent and the facilitation of running time during the reconditioning period. From these results, it might be concluded that a similar induction phenomenon was found in the T-maze as was in the semi-circular maze.
    Experiment II: Eighteen hungry rats were divided equally into two groups (Group Exp. and Group Cont.). After exploration and pretraining, all Ss were given 11 successive free-choice reinforced trials per day for 4 days in a cross-maze with a black arm and a white arm (the conditioning period). On a half of any two consecutive trials, they started from the same starting boxes, and on the other half trials, they started from the opposite starting boxes. Immediately after 41 nonreinforced trials, Group Exp. was given 11 reinforced trials. The Group Cont. did not receive any nonreinforced trial. From the data on their spontaneous alternations, it would be concluded that Ss alternated their choices on the exteroceptive cues during the conditioning period. The Group Exp. showed the dramatic decrement of alternation in percent during the reconditioning period. Their choices were also based on the exteroceptive cues. The running responses of the Group Exp. during this period were significantly faster than the ones of the Group Cont..
  • 望月 享子
    1969 年 39 巻 6 号 p. 308-313
    発行日: 1969/02/10
    公開日: 2010/07/16
    ジャーナル フリー
  • 青木 孝悦
    1969 年 39 巻 6 号 p. 314-318
    発行日: 1969/02/10
    公開日: 2010/07/16
    ジャーナル フリー
  • 言語条件づけ研究会
    1969 年 39 巻 6 号 p. 319-334
    発行日: 1969/02/10
    公開日: 2010/07/16
    ジャーナル フリー
  • 1969 年 39 巻 6 号 p. 335-338
    発行日: 1969/02/10
    公開日: 2010/07/16
    ジャーナル フリー
feedback
Top