心理学研究
Online ISSN : 1884-1082
Print ISSN : 0021-5236
ISSN-L : 0021-5236
88 巻, 1 号
選択された号の論文の10件中1~10を表示しています
原著論文
  • 曹 蓮, 杉森 伸吉, 高 史明
    2017 年 88 巻 1 号 p. 1-10
    発行日: 2017年
    公開日: 2017/04/25
    [早期公開] 公開日: 2017/01/14
    ジャーナル フリー

    In this study, we investigated cultural differences in multisensory perception of emotion between Chinese and Japanese participants, focusing on mutual interference of visual and auditory emotional information. In this experiment, the face-voice pairs were consisted of congruent or incongruent emotions (e.g., a happy (an angry) face with a happy (an angry) voice in congruent pairs, and a happy (an angry) face with an angry (a happy) voice in incongruent pairs). Participants were asked to judge the emotion of targets focusing on either face or voice while ignoring the other modality’s information. In the voice-focus condition, the effect of to-be-ignored facial information was smaller in Japanese than Chinese participants, only when the participant and the target belonged to the same cultures (in-group). This indicated that Japanese people were more likely to be based on the voice information in multisensory perception of emotion of in-group. Our study illuminated that although both Japanese and Chinese people belonged to the Eastern culture, there were cultural differences in perceiving emotion from visual and auditory cues.

  • 藤原 佑貴, 宮寺 貴之, 久原 恵理子, 小林 寿一
    2017 年 88 巻 1 号 p. 11-20
    発行日: 2017年
    公開日: 2017/04/25
    [早期公開] 公開日: 2017/01/14
    ジャーナル フリー

    The effects of two introductory styles on substantive information provided by Japanese children were investigated. Children aged 5–6 years (n = 42) and 7–8 years (n = 40) took a simulated physical examination. One week later, the participants were interviewed about the examination in a narrative introductory condition using open-ended questions, or a non-narrative introductory condition using directive and yes/no questions. Substantive information provided by the children in the two conditions was compared. Results indicated that in the narrative condition, the participants provided more accurate information about the examination in response to open-ended questions. Additionally, the 7–8 year-old children in the narrative condition provided a wider range of critical information. These findings suggest that an open-ended introductory style is effective in eliciting more accurate information from children, including preschoolers, and more varied information from school-age children.

  • 高橋 晃
    2017 年 88 巻 1 号 p. 21-31
    発行日: 2017年
    公開日: 2017/04/25
    [早期公開] 公開日: 2017/01/14
    ジャーナル フリー

    This study investigated the independence of old and new item processing in recognition judgment. Conventional recognition studies have focused on the “memory” aspects of recognition judgment, while new item processing has been treated as a “residual response” of old item processing. However, prior studies on confidence ratings and the brain research suggest the possibility that old and new items are independently processed. This study conducted recognition judgment tests with the number of learned items as independent variables, and response times for four answer categories (hit, miss, correct rejection, and false alarm) as dependent variables. Hit response times were consistently shorter than misses for all old items, while correct rejection (CR) and false alarm (FA) response times approached equivalence (CR = FA) for new items as the number of old (memorized) items increased. These results suggest that recognition judgment changes according to the number of old (memorized) items. We discuss the idea that new item and old item processing in recognition judgment occur independently.

  • 田崎 勝也, 申 知元
    2017 年 88 巻 1 号 p. 32-42
    発行日: 2017年
    公開日: 2017/04/25
    [早期公開] 公開日: 2017/01/14
    ジャーナル フリー

    This study attempts to identify response styles of Japanese students by cross-level and cross-national analyses. Tasaki and Ninomiya (2013) demonstrated the existence of an acquiescence response behavior of Japanese students based on the model by Billet and McClendon(2000), who measured latent response characteristics as “style factor.” In this study, we used a different response style measurement model proposed by Weijters, Schillewaert, & Geuens (2008) positing that response styles are psychological constructs and observed response style variables are summary indicators derived from sets of various, but independent, test items. Results of cross-national analyses indicated that Japanese and Korean participants were more likely to choose a mid-point response (MRS) and less likely to choose an acquiescence response (ARS) than American counterparts, but no significant difference was observed for an extreme response (ERS) among participants from the three nations. Within the Japanese response style model, cross-level analyses showed that ARS scores were significantly higher than ERS scores, indicating that Japanese participants had an acquiescence response style, consistent with the findings from Tasaki & Ninomiya (2013).

    Editor's pick

    2018年度 心理学研究掲載 優秀論文賞受賞論文

    授賞理由
    リカート尺度を用いた質問では,質問の内容に関係なく,特定の選択肢を選択するレスポンス・セット(以下RS)と呼ばれるバイアスの存在が指摘されている。代表的なRSに,極端な選択肢を選ぶ極端反応傾向,中間評価を好んで選ぶ中間反応傾向,内容を十分に吟味することなく項目に同意する黙従反応傾向がある。これまでの研究から,日本人の極端な回答を避け中間的な回答を好む反応傾向が明らかにされている。一方,黙従反応傾向については,まだ研究の数が少なく,結果が定まっていなかった。本論文では,構造方程式モデリングを用いて日米韓の3国を比較・分析することによって,日本人のRSの特徴を検討することが試みられた。分析の結果,日本人の中間回答を好むRSの特徴が再確認された。黙従反応傾向は3国共に高いことも示された。これは,3種のRSの中で黙従反応傾向が最も顕著な回答バイアスであることを意味している。また,多様な文化的価値観に理解を示すバイカルチャー者は黙従反応傾向が強いことから,文化的価値の多様性が黙従反応傾向の規定因の1つであると論じられた。高度な分析手法を用い,日本人のRSの特徴を明らかにした本論文の学術的価値は高い。細部にまで言及した丁寧な考察も評価された。リカート尺度を用いた心理学的研究は極めて多い。今後,心理学研究者はRSに対する理解を深め,バイアス低減に努めることが求められるであろうし,本論文は,その手助けとなる代表的な研究の1つとなることが予想される。以上の点から,本論文は優秀論文賞に値すると判断された。

  • 樋口 収, 埴田 健司
    2017 年 88 巻 1 号 p. 43-50
    発行日: 2017年
    公開日: 2017/04/25
    [早期公開] 公開日: 2017/01/14
    ジャーナル フリー

    Consumers have had concerns over the safety of Fukushima-produced foods since the Fukushima-Daiichi nuclear power plant accident. To dispel these concerns, the public administration has distributed the informational leaflets, which guarantee the safety of Fukushima-produced foods in the marketplace. We investigated the effectiveness of the leaflets. Previous research showed that the activation of behavioral immune system exacerbated prejudice toward out-group members. Therefore, we investigated whether reading the leaflets about the safety of foods would increase prejudice toward foreigners. Participants (N = 50) were asked to read a leaflet either relevant or irrelevant to the safety of Fukushima-produced foods and then complete a Japanese-Foreigners Implicit Association Test and Perceived Vulnerability to Disease Scale. As predicted, participants high in chronic germ aversion (GA) were more prejudiced against foreigners when reading the leaflet relevant to the safety of Fukushima-produced foods than when reading the leaflet irrelevant to the issue. No such effect was observed among participants low in GA. These results indicated the possibility that the current leaflet about the safety of Fukushima foods might backfire.

  • 山口 剛
    2017 年 88 巻 1 号 p. 51-60
    発行日: 2017年
    公開日: 2017/04/25
    [早期公開] 公開日: 2017/01/14
    ジャーナル フリー

    Previous studies have shown a positive correlation between the use of a learning strategy and perceived benefits. However, the impact of perceived benefits as conditional knowledge in metacognitive knowledge has not been examined. The present study investigated the effect of perceived benefits about when (short and long) and how (persistent and suitable) to use learning strategies using two surveys. Each survey utilized a sample of undergraduate students from three different Japanese universities, and the Bayesian hierarchical modeling and within-person variance were used to verify the relationship between the use of a learning strategy and perceived benefits. The students completed questionnaires regarding cognitive (Survey 2a) and metacognitive (Survey 2b) strategies (Survey 1 included two strategies), and achievement goals (without Survey 1). Findings revealed the effects of long-term and short-term persistent perceived benefits on the use of a learning strategy in all the surveys. These results suggest that persistent benefits be emphasized in order to promote the use of learning strategies, and that the appropriate perceived benefits be emphasized in order to become an adaptive learner.

研究資料
  • 日道 俊之, 小山内 秀和, 後藤 崇志, 藤田 弥世, 河村 悠太, Davis, Mark H., 野村 理朗
    2017 年 88 巻 1 号 p. 61-71
    発行日: 2017年
    公開日: 2017/04/25
    [早期公開] 公開日: 2017/01/14
    ジャーナル フリー

    Empathy is a multi-dimensional concept with emotional and cognitive components. The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) is a multi-dimensional scale of empathic traits. Although some researchers have attempted to translate the IRI into Japanese, these translated scales had limitations with content and construct validity, and measurement invariance. We therefore attempt to overcome these limitations by developing a new Japanese version of the IRI (IRI-J). We used three approaches to assess the validity and measurement invariance of the IRI-J. In Study 1, content validity was tested using back-translation, and construct validity was confirmed through a comprehensive investigation of a web-based survey using six other scales. Results indicate that the factor structure of the IRI-J was equivalent to that of the original version, and that the IRI-J had adequate reliability and construct validity. In Study 2, measurement invariance by gender was confirmed using data from four web-based surveys. These results suggest that the factor model of IRI-J for each gender is equivalent. The present study thus provides an improved measure of empathic traits for the Japanese population.

研究報告
  • 竹ヶ原 靖子, 安保 英勇
    2017 年 88 巻 1 号 p. 72-78
    発行日: 2017年
    公開日: 2017/04/25
    [早期公開] 公開日: 2017/01/14
    ジャーナル フリー

    The present study examined effects of predictions made by a help-seeker about the emotions and costs of a helper on help-seeking behavior. We surveyed undergraduate and graduate students who completed a questionnaire and scenario task. The scenario presented that a person would face the trouble. The questionnaire presented a case where a person was troubled. Respondents were required to answer the following questions: (a) whether they would seek help; (b) whether the helper would respond to help-seeking; (c) the helper’s costs; (d) the negative moods of the helper. Covariance structure analysis of 189 participant responses revealed that help-seeker predictions about the helper’s emotions and costs were used to predict the intentions of the helper, which affected the intentions to seek help. We discuss these results with respect to interpersonal cognition in intimate relationships.

  • 伊藤 直樹
    2017 年 88 巻 1 号 p. 79-85
    発行日: 2017年
    公開日: 2017/04/25
    [早期公開] 公開日: 2017/01/14
    ジャーナル フリー

    This study aimed to compare information provided on student counseling center websites of universities and colleges in Japan, the United States, the United Kingdom, and Taiwan. A survey was conducted on websites of 315 centers in Japan, 282 centers in the United States, 70 centers in the United Kingdom and 61 centers in Taiwan. Trends in the provision of information on websites in each country were analyzed and compared for the rate and quantity of information published. Results of multiple correspondence analyses indicated two basic dimensions of information that could effectively distinguish information provided in the four countries. These were provision of necessary information and provision of information for use of individual counseling or support of community. Finally, issues related to websites in student counseling centers of Japanese universities and colleges are discussed.

  • 梅本 貴豊, 田中 健史朗
    2017 年 88 巻 1 号 p. 86-92
    発行日: 2017年
    公開日: 2017/04/25
    [早期公開] 公開日: 2017/01/14
    ジャーナル フリー

    This study examined the relationships among motivational regulation strategies, motivational factors, and learning behaviors outside the classroom. There are three subtypes of motivational regulation strategies: autonomous regulation strategies, cooperative strategies, and performance-focused strategies. Motivational factors included in the investigation were self-efficacy and task value, while behavioral and emotional engagement and study time were selected as learning behaviors outside the classroom. A self-report questionnaire was administered to 322 undergraduates from two universities. Multiple regression analysis revealed the use of autonomous regulation strategies, and that task value was positively correlated with engagement and study time. Moreover, self-efficacy positively predicted study time. In contrast, the use of performance strategies negatively predicted engagement. The use of cooperative strategies did not predict learning behaviors. These results indicate that motivation, as well as the regulation of motivation, were important for learning outside the classroom. The effects of regulation of motivation and motivation on learning outside the classroom are discussed in light of the current findings.

feedback
Top